Chapter 3 – Language Development in Children
Chapter 3 focuses on the stages of language acquisition and the factors that influence language development. The chapter also discusses the critical period hypothesis, which suggests that there is a biologically determined window of opportunity for language acquisition, and the importance of interactional modifications and feedback in second language acquisition. The chapter begins by discussing the stages of language acquisition, which include the prelinguistic stage, the babbling stage, the one-word stage, and the two-word stage. These stages represent the natural progression of language development in children, and they are influenced by a variety of factors, including genetic, environmental, and social factors. The critical period hypothesis is also discussed in this chapter, which proposes that there is a specific period in early childhood during which language acquisition occurs most easily and efficiently, and after which language learning becomes more difficult. The chapter explains the critical period hypothesis in detail and explores the implications of this hypothesis for language acquisition and age.
The chapter also highlights the importance of interactional modifications in language learning. Interactional modifications refer to the changes that occur in the way people communicate when they are trying to accommodate the language needs of their interlocutors. These modifications can take many forms, such as simplifying language, repeating information, or using gestures to convey meaning. Feedback is another important factor in language development, and the chapter discusses different forms of feedback, including corrective feedback and recasts. The chapter provides examples of how feedback can be used to support language learning and improve language skills.
To help learners understand these concepts, the chapter includes a case study and problem-solving exercises. The case study presents a scenario in which a child is struggling with language development, and learners are asked to analyze the factors that may be contributing to the child’s difficulties. The problem-solving exercises provide learners with the opportunity to apply what they have learned and develop strategies for supporting language development in children.
Guiding questions for discussion
- What are the factors that influence language development?
- How does age affect language acquisition in children and adults?
- What is the critical period hypothesis in language learning?
- How do social networks influence language learning?
- What are the different types of feedback in language development?
A Case study
Case study: Improving Second Language Acquisition through Social Interaction
Background: Maria is a 16-year-old student who recently moved to a new country and is struggling to learn the language. Despite attending language classes, she finds it difficult to communicate with her peers and make friends. Her parents are concerned that her lack of social interaction is hindering her language development.
Problem: How can Maria improve her second language acquisition through social interaction?
Introduction
Language development in children is a complex process that occurs from birth to adolescence. It involves the acquisition of speech sounds, words, grammar, and the ability to use language in social contexts. The development of language skills is critical for communication, learning, and social interaction. This response will provide an overview of language development in children, including the stages of language acquisition, factors that influence language development, and interventions for language delays.
Stages of Language Acquisition: Language acquisition occurs in predictable stages. The first stage, the prelinguistic stage, occurs from birth to around 0 -6 and 6- 12 months of age. During this stage, infants develop the ability to produce and understand speech sounds. The next stage, the holophrastic or one-word stage, occurs from 12 to 18 months of age. During this stage, children use single words to communicate their needs and wants. The two-word stage occurs from 18 to 24 months of age. During this stage, children begin to combine two words to form simple sentences. The telegraphic stage occurs from 24 to 30 months of age. Children begin to use more complex sentence structures and include grammatical markers during this stage. The final stage, the mature stage, occurs from 30 months of age and continues into adolescence. During this stage, children have a more advanced understanding of grammar and can use language in complex social contexts (Hoff, 2013).
Factors that Influence Language Development: Several factors influence language development in children. These include genetic factors, environmental factors, and social factors. Genetic factors can influence language development by affecting the development of the brain structures responsible for language processing. Environmental factors, such as exposure to language, can influence language development by providing opportunities for children to hear and practice language. Social factors, such as the quality of interactions with caregivers, can also influence language development by providing opportunities for children to learn language in a social context (Hart & Risley, 1995).
Interventions for Language Delays: Language delays can occur for various reasons, including hearing impairment, developmental delays, and environmental factors. Interventions for language delays include speech therapy, language therapy, and parent-implemented interventions. Speech therapy and language therapy involve working with a trained therapist to develop language skills. Parent-implemented interventions involve training parents to use language-rich interactions with their children to promote language development (Law et al., 2003).
Language development in children is a complex process that occurs in predictable stages. Several factors influence language development, including genetic, environmental, and social factors. Interventions for language delays include speech therapy, language therapy, and parent-implemented interventions. Understanding the stages of language development and the factors that influence it is essential for promoting healthy language development in children.
Stages of Language Acquisition
Stage 1: Pre-linguistic Stage
Language acquisition occurs in predictable stages. The first stage, the prelinguistic stage, occurs from birth to around 6 months of age. The prelinguistic stage is the initial stage of language development that occurs from birth to approximately 6 months of age. During this stage, infants develop the ability to produce and understand speech sounds, laying the foundation for future language development (Hoff, 2013). Infants communicate through nonverbal means such as crying, cooing, and gestures. They learn to distinguish between sounds and tones of their language and can recognize their parents’ voices.
During the pre-linguistic stage, infants engage in communication through nonverbal means, such as facial expressions, body movements, and vocalizations. Crying, for example, is a form of communication that can signal discomfort, hunger, or pain. Cooing and babbling are also common during this stage, and infants use them for experimenting with the sounds of their language (O’Grady et al., 2019). For instance, infants start to produce vowel-like sounds, such as “oo” and “ah,” and try to replicate the intonation patterns of the language they hear around them (Kuhl, 2010). Moreover, infants learn to distinguish between different sounds and tones of their language, even if they do not yet understand their meaning. For example, they can recognize their parents’ voices and distinguish between a happy or angry tone of voice (DeCasper & Fifer, 1980).
Infants also communicate through gestures, such as pointing, reaching, and waving. These gestures are essential for their social and cognitive development, as they allow infants to interact with their environment and to communicate their needs and interests to others (Goldin-Meadow & Butcher, 2003). For example, infants may point to a toy they want to play with or reach out to their parent to be picked up. They learn to distinguish between sounds and tones of their language and can recognize their parents’ voices. These early forms of communication are crucial for infants’ social and cognitive development and set the foundation for language acquisition.
Infants in the prelinguistic stage also begin to develop the ability to understand language, even if they cannot yet produce it. Infants are able to discriminate between speech sounds from different languages and can recognize their own name and familiar words, such as “mommy” and “daddy” (Werker & Tees, 2005).
The development of speech sounds during the prelinguistic stage is influenced by various factors, including genetics, environmental factors, and the amount of exposure to language. Infants who are exposed to a greater amount of language tend to produce speech sounds earlier than those who are not exposed to as much language (Kuhl, 2007).
The prelinguistic stage is a critical period in language development, during which infants develop the ability to produce and understand speech sounds. Infants in this stage produce various sounds such as crying, cooing, and babbling, and they also develop the ability to discriminate between speech sounds from different languages. The development of speech sounds in the prelinguistic stage is influenced by various factors, including genetics, environmental factors, and the amount of exposure to language.
Stage 2: Babbling
Stage 2 of first language acquisition is the babbling stage, which typically occurs between the ages of 6 and 12 months. During this stage, infants begin to produce repetitive, meaningless sounds such as “ma-ma” and “da-da.” These sounds are not yet words, but they are important precursors to language development as they allow infants to experiment with the sounds of their language and learn how to produce them. It is an essential stage in language development as it allows infants to experiment with the sounds of their language and learn how to produce them.
Babbling is a universal behavior, and all infants engage in it to some extent (Oller, 2000). Babbling sounds vary depending on the language being learned. For example, infants learning English tend to produce sounds that are similar to “ba-ba” and “da-da,” while infants learning Japanese produce sounds that are more like “pa-pa” and “ta-ta” (Ota & Saji, 2006). This suggests that even at this early stage, infants are beginning to tune into their language’s specific sounds and patterns.
Research has shown that babbling serves an important role in language development. Infants who babble more frequently and produce a wider range of sounds tend to develop language skills more quickly than those who babble less (Oller, 2000). This suggests that the more infants practice producing sounds, the more adept they become at producing and recognizing the sounds of their language.
The babbling stage is an important precursor to language development. Infants experiment with the sounds of their language during this stage, producing repetitive, meaningless sounds such as “ma-ma” and “da-da.” Babbling is important in language development and helps infants develop their language skills.
Stage 3: One-Word Stage
The third stage, the holophrastic or one-word stage, occurs from 12 to 18 months of age. During this stage, children begin to use single words to communicate their needs and wants (Hoff, 2013). During this stage, children begin to use one-word utterances to communicate. These words usually refer to people, objects, or actions that are familiar to them, such as “mama” to refer to their mother or “juice” to ask for a drink. Children use single words to convey entire thoughts or messages. For example, a child may say “ball” to indicate that they want to play with a ball or “up” to indicate that they want to be picked up. While these one-word utterances are not yet complete sentences, they allow children to express their needs and desires more effectively than crying or pointing alone.
In the holophrastic stage, children use a single word to convey a complete thought or idea. For example, a child might say “milk” to indicate that they want a glass of milk or “ball” to indicate that they want to play with a ball. These single words are often accompanied by gestures, such as pointing or reaching, to help convey their intended message (Hoff, 2013).
It is important to note that children are not limited to just using nouns during this stage. They may also use verbs or adjectives to convey meaning. For example, a child might say “go” to indicate that they want to go outside or “hot” to indicate that they do not want to eat food that is too hot (Hoff, 2013).
The use of single words in the holophrastic stage is significant because it marks the transition from using sounds to communicate to using words with specific meanings. Children at this stage have a basic understanding of the meaning of words and how they can use them to communicate with others (Hoff, 2013).
The development of vocabulary during the holophrastic stage is influenced by various factors, including the child’s exposure to language ancognitive and social development. Children exposed to a greater amount of language tend to have a larger vocabulary than those not exposed to as much language (Fenson et al., 1994). Additionally, children who have more opportunities for social interaction tend to have more developed language skills (Huttenlocher et al., 2010).
The holophrastic or one-word stage is a critical period in language development during which children begin to use single words to communicate their needs and wants. The use of single words is significant because it marks the transition from using sounds to communicate to using words with specific meanings. The development of vocabulary during this stage is influenced by various factors, including the child’s exposure to language and their cognitive and social development.
During the one-word stage, children also begin to acquire an understanding of the grammatical rules of their language. For example, they may use the -ing suffix to indicate ongoing actions, such as saying “eating” while they are in the process of eating. They may also begin to use plural forms, such as saying “dogs” to refer to more than one dog.
Research has shown that the one-word stage is an important milestone in language development. Children who reach this stage are well on their way to developing more complex language skills (Bloom, 1993). Parents can help facilitate their children’s language development during this stage by talking to them frequently and using simple, clear language.
The one-word stage is an important stage in first language acquisition. Children begin to use single words to convey entire thoughts or messages, allowing them to express their needs and desires more effectively. This stage also marks the beginning of children’s understanding of the grammatical rules of their language.
Stage 4: Two-Word Stage
The two-word stage, also known as the telegraphic stage, is the third stage of language development and occurs between 18 and 24 months of age (Fenson et al., 1994). During this stage, children begin to combine two words to form simple sentences, although they may not yet use all the necessary grammatical structures to produce complete sentences.
Examples of two-word utterances might include “mommy book,” “daddy car,” or “more juice.” These utterances typically consist of a noun and a verb or an adjective and a noun, and they convey basic information about what the child wants or needs. Between 18 to 24 months, children begin to combine two words to form simple phrases. These phrases typically consist of nouns and verbs, such as “baby sleep” or “daddy go.”
The two-word stage is also known as the telegraphic stage, as children’s language resembles a telegram with only the essential words included. While these phrases may not yet be grammatically correct or complete sentences, they allow children to express a wider range of ideas and concepts.
As children progress through the two-word stage, they also begin to acquire a more extensive vocabulary. They may start to use adjectives, such as “big dog” or “pretty flower,” to describe the objects they are referring to. They may also use prepositions, such as “on” or “in,” to describe the location of objects or people.
Research has shown that the two-word stage is an important milestone in language development. Children who reach this stage demonstrate an increasing understanding of the grammatical rules of their language, as well as an expanding vocabulary (Bloom, 1993). Parents can help facilitate their children’s language development during this stage by engaging in conversation with them, asking them open-ended questions, and introducing new words and concepts.
The two-word stage is an important stage in first language acquisition. Children combine two words to form simple phrases, allowing them to express a wider range of ideas and concepts. This stage also marks an increasing understanding of the grammatical rules of their language and an expanding vocabulary.
The telegraphic speech of the two-word stage reflects the child’s emerging understanding of grammar and sentence structure. Children at this stage have developed the ability to use word order to convey meaning, and they understand that different word orders can change the meaning of a sentence (Bloom & Markson, 1998).
The development of the two-word stage is influenced by various factors, including the child’s exposure to language, their cognitive and social development, and their individual temperament and personality (Huttenlocher et al., 2010). Children exposed to a greater variety of words and sentence structures tend to develop their language skills more quickly than those not exposed to as much language (Hoff, 2013).
It is important to note that while the two-word stage is a significant milestone in language development, children may not yet use all of the grammatical structures required to produce complete sentences. For example, they may not yet use articles like “the” or “a,” and they may not use verb tenses correctly. However, they are beginning to use more complex language and to form sentences that convey more information than simple one-word utterances.
The two-word stage is an important milestone in language development during which children begin to combine two words to form simple sentences. While children at this stage may not yet use all of the grammatical structures necessary for complete sentences, they are developing their understanding of grammar and sentence structure. The development of the two-word stage is influenced by various factors, including the child’s exposure to language, their cognitive and social development, and their individual temperament and personality.
Stage 5: Telegraphic Stage
The fifth stage, the telegraphic stage, occurs from 24 to 30 months of age. The telegraphic stage, also known as the early multiword stage, is the fourth stage of language development and occurs between 24 and 30 months of age (Bates et al., 1988). During this stage, children begin to use more complex sentence structures and include grammatical markers, such as articles, prepositions, and verb tense, to convey meaning.
Stage 5 of first language acquisition is the telegraphic stage, which typically occurs around two years of age. During this stage, children begin to produce short sentences that consist of only essential words. These sentences are called telegraphic because they resemble telegrams, which also typically contain only essential words.
Examples of telegraphic sentences include “me want cookie” or “doggie bark.” These sentences are not grammatically correct and are missing articles, prepositions, and other grammatical markers. However, they are still effective at conveying the child’s intended meaning.
At this stage, children’s language development is characterized by an increasing ability to use grammatical rules and combine words to form longer sentences. They may also use more complex sentence structures, such as asking questions or using negation. For example, a child may say “where mommy go?” or “no want to eat.”
The telegraphic stage marks an important milestone in language development as children begin to use language to express their thoughts and ideas more fully. It also marks a transition from using single words or two-word phrases to using longer, more complex sentences. As children continue to develop their language skills, they will gradually refine their grammar and expand their vocabulary.
Research has shown that children acquire important language skills during the telegraphic stage, such as word order, verb tense, and subject-verb agreement (Brown, 1973). Parents and caregivers can support children’s language development during this stage by providing opportunities for them to practice using language, engaging in conversations with them, and introducing new vocabulary and concepts.
The telegraphic stage is important in first language acquisition, during which children begin to produce short sentences with only essential words. While these sentences are not grammatically correct, they are effective at conveying the child’s intended meaning. As children continue to develop their language skills, they will gradually refine their grammar and expand their vocabulary.
The emergence of grammatical markers during the telegraphic stage is a significant milestone in language development, as it reflects the child’s growing understanding of grammar and sentence structure. Children at this stage have developed the ability to use grammatical markers to indicate relationships between words, and they understand how different word orders can affect the meaning of a sentence (Hoff, 2013).
The development of the telegraphic stage is influenced by various factors, including the child’s exposure to language, their cognitive and social development, and their individual temperament and personality (Bloom, 1994). Children exposed to a greater variety of sentence structures and grammatical markers tend to develop their language skills more quickly than those not exposed to as much language (Huttenlocher et al., 2010).
It is important to note that while children at the telegraphic stage are beginning to use more complex language and sentence structures, they may still make grammar and word order errors. For example, they may omit function words like “is” or “the” or use verb tense inconsistently (Bates et al., 1988). However, these errors are a natural part of the language learning process and typically diminish with continued exposure to language and feedback from caregivers.
The telegraphic stage is an important milestone in language development during which children begin to use more complex sentence structures and include grammatical markers. Children at this stage have developed the ability to use grammatical markers to convey meaning and understand how different word orders can affect the meaning of a sentence. The development of the telegraphic stage is influenced by various factors, including the child’s exposure to language, their cognitive and social development, and their individual temperament and personality.
Stage 6: Language Consolidation or mature language
The final stage of language development is language consolidation or the mature stage, which begins around 30 months of age and continues into adolescence. During this stage, children have a more advanced understanding of grammar and can use language in complex social contexts (Hoff, 2013). Children at this stage have acquired most of their grammatical knowledge and become proficient communicators. Children’s language development becomes more advanced as they begin to use more complex sentences and show an understanding of grammar rules (Clark, 2003). Children begin to use language for social purposes such as expressing emotions, making requests, and telling stories (Bloom, 1993).
For example, a child may say “I feel sad” to express their emotions, “Can I have some water, please?” to make a request, or “Once upon a time, there was a big, bad wolf” to tell a story. They also begin to use more advanced grammatical structures such as past tense and plurals.
During this stage, children also develop metalinguistic awareness, which is the ability to think and talk about language itself (Goswami, 2001). They become more aware of the nuances of language, such as the use of sarcasm and irony, and begin to understand the different registers of language used in different contexts, such as formal versus informal language.
In the mature stage, children have an understanding of the complexities of syntax, morphology, and semantics. They are able to use language to convey abstract ideas and engage in discussions about topics such as politics, philosophy, and literature. Their vocabulary continues to expand, and they develop the ability to use figurative language, such as idioms and metaphors (Bates et al., 1988).
Examples of language use in the mature stage might include the use of complex sentence structures, such as subordinate clauses and passive voice. Children may also use language to express their opinions and persuade others to adopt their point of view. For example, a child might argue persuasively in a debate about the benefits of vegetarianism versus meat-eating.
Children at the mature stage also develop the ability to use language in a variety of social contexts. They learn how to adjust their language use to different situations and audiences, such as formal language in academic settings or informal language with friends (Hoff, 2013). They also learn how to use nonverbal cues, such as tone of voice and facial expressions, to convey meaning and express emotion. They start to use more complex sentences and show an understanding of grammar rules. They also begin to use language for social purposes such as expressing emotions, making requests, and telling stories.
The language consolidation stage is a critical phase in a child’s language development as they continue to refine and expand their language skills, preparing them for more advanced forms of communication and literacy (Clark, 2003). The development of the mature stage is influenced by a variety of factors, including the child’s exposure to language, their cognitive and social development, and their educational experiences (Bates et al., 1988). Children who are exposed to a wide variety of language and social experiences tend to develop stronger language skills than those who are not.
In brief, the mature stage is the final stage of language development and occurs from around 30 months of age into adolescence. During this stage, children have a more advanced understanding of grammar and can use language in complex social contexts. They are able to express abstract ideas, use complex sentence structures, and adjust their language use to different situations and audiences. The development of the mature stage is influenced by a variety of factors, including exposure to language, cognitive and social development, and educational experiences.
Factors that Influence Language Development
Language development is a complex process that is influenced by various factors. These factors can be broadly categorized into genetic, environmental, and social factors. Genetic factors play an important role in language development. Environmental factors such as exposure to language can also influence language development. Children who are exposed to language-rich environments, such as those with parents who talk to them frequently, tend to have better language skills. In contrast, children who grow up in language-poor environments, such as those with parents who do not talk to them much, may have delayed language development. Social factors, such as the quality of interactions with caregivers, can also have an impact on language development. Children who have frequent, positive interactions with caregivers tend to have better language skills than those who do not. This may be because such interactions provide opportunities for children to learn language in a social context. A combination of genetic, environmental, and social factors all play a role in language development. Understanding these factors can help parents, educators, and healthcare providers support children’s language development and ensure that they have the skills they need for success in school and beyond.
Genetic factors
Language development is influenced by a variety of factors, including genetic factors. Research has shown that genetics can play a role in language development, as language ability is known to run in families (Bishop, 2002). Studies have also found that genetic factors contribute to the development of language-related skills, such as phonological processing and verbal memory (Bishop & Snowling, 2004).
One of the genes that has been linked to language development is the FOXP2 gene. This gene is involved in the development and functioning of the brain and has been shown to play a role in language ability (Vargha-Khadem et al., 2005). Mutations in the FOXP2 gene have been linked to language impairments, such as specific language impairment (SLI) and developmental apraxia of speech (DAS) (Vargha-Khadem et al., 2005).
Another gene that has been linked to language development is the CNTNAP2 gene. This gene is involved in the development of the neural connections in the brain and has been shown to be associated with language ability, as well as with reading and writing ability (Scerri et al., 2011).
While genetic factors can play a role in language development, it is important to note that the influence of genetics is complex and multifaceted. Language development is also influenced by environmental factors, such as exposure to language and social experiences (Bates et al., 1988). In fact, research has shown that the interaction between genetic and environmental factors is important in determining language outcomes (Plomin & Kennedy, 1999).
Genetic factors can play a role in language development, and certain genes have been linked to language-related skills and impairments. However, the influence of genetics is complex, and language development is also influenced by environmental factors. Understanding the interplay between genetic and environmental factors is important for understanding individual differences in language development.
Environmental factors
Environmental factors play an important role in language development, as they can have a significant impact on the quality and quantity of language input that children receive. The following are some examples of environmental factors that can influence language development:
- Parental language input: The amount and quality of language input that children receive from their parents and caregivers can have a significant impact on their language development. Children who are exposed to rich and varied language input are more likely to develop strong language skills than children who are exposed to limited or impoverished language input (Hart & Risley, 1995).
- Quantity of language: The amount of language that children hear can influence their language development. Research has shown that children who are exposed to more language tend to have larger vocabularies and better language skills (Hart & Risley, 1995).
- Quality of language: The quality of language input that children receive can also influence their language development. Children who are exposed to language that is rich in vocabulary and grammatical structure tend to have stronger language skills (Hoff, 2003).
- Socioeconomic status: Children from low-income families are often exposed to less language input than children from higher-income families, which can put them at a disadvantage in terms of language development (Hoff, 2003).
- Cultural background: The cultural background of children and their families can also influence language development. For example, some cultures place a greater emphasis on verbal communication than others, and this can impact the language development of children from those cultures (García-Sierra et al., 2011).
- Educational opportunities: The quality of educational opportunities that children receive can also influence language development. Children who attend high-quality early childhood education programs, for example, are more likely to develop strong language skills than children who do not have access to such programs (Huttenlocher et al., 2010).
- Parental involvement: The level of parental involvement in children’s language development can also be an important environmental factor. Parents who actively engage with their children in language-rich activities, such as reading and storytelling, can help support their children’s language development (Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2002).
Environmental factors can have a significant impact on language development in children. Parental language input, socioeconomic status, cultural background, educational opportunities, and parental involvement are all examples of environmental factors that can influence language development.
Social factors
Social factors play an important role in language development, as social interactions provide children with opportunities to practice and refine their language skills. The following are some examples of social factors that can influence language development:
- Peer interactions: Children’s interactions with their peers can help support their language development by providing opportunities for conversation and social play (Dunn & Kontos, 1997).
- Parent-child interactions: Positive interactions between parents and children can help support language development. For example, parents who engage in responsive and contingent communication with their children can help support their children’s language skills (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2014).
- Parenting style: The overall parenting style can also influence language development. For example, children of parents who use more positive reinforcement and less punishment tend to have better language skills (Bornstein & Tamis-LeMonda, 1989).
- Conversational turns: Children who engage in more conversational turns with caregivers tend to have better language skills (Weisleder & Fernald, 2013). This can include back-and-forth exchanges of language, such as questions and answers, comments, and shared attention to objects.
- Cultural norms: Cultural norms regarding language use and communication can also influence language development. For example, in some cultures, children are encouraged to speak and communicate freely, while in other cultures, children are expected to be more reserved in their communication (Ochs & Schieffelin, 2012).
- Media exposure: The use of electronic media can also influence language development. Children who are exposed to high-quality educational media may benefit from increased exposure to language-rich content (Zimmerman et al., 2007).
- Language exposure: Children who are exposed to multiple languages may experience different language learning opportunities, depending on factors such as the amount of exposure to each language and the quality of the language input (Byers-Heinlein & Lew-Williams, 2013).
Social factors can have a significant impact on language development in children. Peer interactions, parent-child interactions, cultural norms, media exposure, and language exposure are all examples of social factors that can influence language development.
Watch this video clip on YouTube about Why learning needs a social context
Language acquisition and age: critical period hypothesis
Research has shown that age plays a significant role in the process of learning a second language, as there are specific periods when language acquisition is most effective. According to the critical period hypothesis, there is a biologically determined window of opportunity for language acquisition, during which it is easiest and most effective to learn a language (Lenneberg, 1967). Thus, understanding the relationship between age and language acquisition is crucial for successful language learning. Here are some important points regarding age and critical periods in language learning.
The ages of 0-3 years old
During the first three years of life, children experience rapid and significant development in their language abilities. At birth, infants are able to distinguish between speech sounds from all languages, but over time they become more attuned to the specific sounds of the language(s) they are exposed to (Kuhl et al., 2006). Infants begin to babble around 6 months of age, producing a range of sounds that resemble speech, and by 10 to 12 months, they begin to produce their first words (Fernald et al., 2008).
Research suggests that children between the ages of 0-3 years old have an enhanced ability to learn language due to their developing neural and cognitive systems (Kuhl et al., 2014). This is because their brains are highly plastic and adaptable, allowing them to more easily form new neural connections and learn new information (Grossmann et al., 2010). Additionally, during this period, children are highly motivated to learn language in order to communicate their wants and needs, and to understand the world around them (Fernald et al., 2008).
Furthermore, during this period, children are able to acquire language skills at a rapid pace and can develop a strong foundation of their native language (Werker & Tees, 2005). Children in this age range are also able to pick up on the nuances of a language more easily and acquire native-like pronunciation (Kuhl et al., 2014). Therefore, it is important for parents and caregivers to provide rich language exposure and opportunities for interaction during this critical period of language development.
The ages of 4-6 years old
Children between the ages of 4-6 years old are in the preschool and kindergarten age range, and they continue to acquire language skills rapidly (Bloom, 1993). During this period, children’s vocabulary expands rapidly, and they begin to use more complex sentence structures. They also start to understand and use grammar rules correctly and produce longer and more detailed narratives.
Children in this age range also have a better understanding of the social context of language use and can use language to initiate and maintain social relationships with their peers (Bloom, 1993). They may engage in conversations with their friends and use language to negotiate and resolve conflicts.
Research has shown that the age range of 4-6 years old is a critical period for the development of certain language skills, such as phonological awareness and literacy (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998). Children who receive early and high-quality literacy experiences during this period have been shown to have better language and reading outcomes later in life (National Early Literacy Panel, 2008).
The ages of 7-12 years old
Between the ages of 7-12 years old, children are still able to acquire language relatively easily, but there may be some decline in their ability to acquire native-like pronunciation (Flege, 1987). Additionally, children in this age range may struggle more with the grammar and syntax of a new language, as their brains are becoming less plastic and more specialized for their first language (Johnson & Newport, 1989). However, with consistent exposure and practice, children in this age range can still make significant progress in acquiring a new language (Gathercole, 2006).
Several studies have examined the effectiveness of second language learning in children between the ages of 7-12 years old. For example, a study by Oller and colleagues (2007) found that children in this age range who received intensive language instruction over the course of several weeks were able to make significant gains in their second language proficiency. Another study by Jia and colleagues (2019) found that children in this age range who were exposed to a new language through immersion programs showed improvements in both their second language skills and their first language skills.
While language acquisition may become slightly more challenging for children between the ages of 7-12 years old, they are still capable of making significant progress in learning a new language with consistent exposure and practice.
The ages of 13-18 years old
During adolescence, language acquisition is still possible, but it becomes more challenging as the brain becomes less plastic and more specialized in certain areas of language processing (Krashen & Scarcella, 1978). However, research suggests that second language acquisition can still be successful during this period with the help of explicit instruction, meaningful interaction, and opportunities for language use (Swain & Lapkin, 2002).
Adolescents have a wider range of cognitive and metacognitive abilities than younger children, which can aid in language learning. They also have more life experiences and background knowledge that can be used to support their language development (Larsen-Freeman & Long, 2014). It is important to note that individual factors such as motivation, personality, and learning style can also play a significant role in language acquisition at any age (Dörnyei, 2005).
Interactional modifications
In second language acquisition (SLA), the nature of interactional modifications is an important factor in language learning. Interactional modifications refer to the changes that occur in the way people communicate when they are trying to accommodate the language needs of their interlocutors. These modifications can take many forms, such as simplifying language, repeating information, or using gestures to convey meaning.
For example, when speaking to a young child who is still developing their language skills, parents may simplify their language to make it more accessible for the child. This could involve using shorter sentences, simpler vocabulary, rephrasing complex sentences, and using gestures or visual aids to help convey meaning.
- Using shorter sentences: Instead of saying “Please go to your room and get your book from the shelf,” a parent might say “Go get your book.”
- Using simpler vocabulary: Instead of saying “The restaurant was very crowded,” a parent might say “There were a lot of people in the restaurant.”
- Rephrasing complex sentences: Instead of saying “The movie was not as good as I thought it would be,” a parent might say “I didn’t like the movie very much.”
- Using gestures or visual aids: A parent might point to objects or use pictures to help their child understand what they are saying.
Studies have shown that interactional modifications can have a positive effect on language learning. For example, one study found that when teachers used more interactional modifications, their students made greater gains in vocabulary acquisition (Lyster & Mori, 2006). Another study found that when learners received feedback in the form of interactional modifications, they were more likely to notice their errors and make corrections (Mackey & Philp, 1998).
However, the nature of interactional modifications is complex and varies depending on the context. Some studies have found that modifications that are too frequent or too explicit can be detrimental to language learning, as they may impede the development of learners’ independent processing skills (Swain, 1995). Additionally, the use of interactional modifications may be influenced by cultural and social factors, such as the norms of politeness and respect in different cultures.
The nature of interactional modifications in SLA is a complex and multifaceted area of study. While research suggests that certain types of modifications can be beneficial for language learning, the optimal frequency and form of these modifications may vary depending on the context and the needs of the learners.
Feedback
Feedback is a crucial aspect of language development, as it provides learners with information about the accuracy and appropriateness of their language use. Feedback can take many forms, including corrective feedback, recasts, clarification requests, and elicitation. Studies have shown that corrective feedback can be effective in promoting language learning, particularly when it is provided in a timely and informative manner (Ellis, 2009). Recasts, for example, involve subtly correcting learners’ errors by repeating their utterances with the error corrected (Lyster & Ranta, 1997). Research has shown that recasts can be effective in promoting accurate language production, particularly when learners are provided with opportunities to notice the correction and when the correction is provided in a contextually appropriate manner (Lyster & Mori, 2006).
Corrective feedback, in particular, has been the focus of much research in second language acquisition (SLA), with scholars investigating its effectiveness in promoting accurate language production and its potential impact on learner motivation and affective states (Mackey & Goo, 2007). Corrective feedback refers to any type of feedback that explicitly indicates an error in a learner’s language use and provides the correct form. This type of feedback can take many forms, such as explicit correction, repetition, or prompting. For example, a teacher might correct a student’s pronunciation of a word or provide explicit feedback on a grammatical error. Corrective feedback can take different forms and may vary depending on the context and the proficiency level of the learner. Some examples of corrective feedback include:
- Explicit correction: This type of feedback involves the teacher or the interlocutor providing an explicit correction of an error made by the learner. For instance, if a learner says “I goed to the store,” the teacher may say “You mean ‘I went to the store’.”
- Implicit correction: This type of feedback involves correcting the error without explicitly pointing it out. For instance, if a learner says “I goed to the store,” the teacher may respond by saying “Oh, you went to the store? What did you buy?”
- Repetition: This type of feedback involves the teacher or the interlocutor repeating the erroneous sentence, emphasizing the correct form. For example, if a learner says “He don’t like pizza,” the teacher may respond by repeating the sentence and emphasizing the correct form, saying “He doesn’t like pizza.”
Recasts are another type of feedback that involves reformulating a learner’s utterance in a way that provides a model of the correct form, without explicitly indicating an error. Recasts can take many different forms, such as repeating a sentence with the correct form, rephrasing a sentence with the correct form, or using a nonverbal cue to indicate the correct form. Recasts are a type of corrective feedback where the teacher or interlocutor repeats the learner’s incorrect utterance with the correct form or structure, without explicitly indicating the error.
Here are some examples of recasts:
- Learner: “I goed to the store yesterday.” Interlocutor: “Oh, you went to the store yesterday.”
- Learner: “She don’t like pizza.” Interlocutor: “She doesn’t like pizza.”
- Learner: “I have two dogs. They is very cute.” Interlocutor: “Oh, you have two dogs. They are very cute.”
In each of these examples, the interlocutor subtly corrects the learner’s error by repeating the incorrect sentence with the correct form or structure. Recasts can help learners notice their errors and make appropriate adjustments to their language production.
Clarification requests, which involve asking learners to clarify their meaning or to repeat their utterance, can also be effective in promoting accurate language use. These requests can help learners notice their errors and provide them with opportunities to self-correct (Long & Robinson, 1998).
Clarification requests are another form of interactional modification that can occur in second language acquisition (SLA). These are requests for clarification when the listener does not understand something that the speaker has said. The speaker can then modify their language use to clarify the message for the listener. Some examples of clarification requests are:
- “I’m sorry, could you repeat that?”
- “I didn’t quite catch what you said. Can you say it again, please?”
- “Can you explain what you mean by ______?”
- “Could you give me an example?”
In SLA, clarification requests are important because they provide learners with feedback on their understanding of the language and help them to identify areas where they need to improve their comprehension or production skills.
Elicitation, in which the teacher prompts learners to produce language, can also be an effective form of feedback. This type of feedback can help learners notice gaps in their knowledge and encourage them to actively engage in the language learning process (van Compernolle & Kormos, 2014).
Elicitation is a technique used by language teachers or conversation partners to prompt learners to produce a particular type of language. This can be done through various methods such as asking questions, providing cues, or modeling language use. Some examples of elicitation techniques are:
- Prompting: A teacher may prompt a student to use a particular word or structure by asking a question or providing a cue. For example, if a teacher wants a student to use the present perfect tense, they may ask “Have you ever traveled outside the country?”
- Modeling: A teacher or conversation partner may model the use of a particular language structure or vocabulary item, and then ask the learner to repeat or use it in a sentence. For example, a teacher may say “I like to swim in the ocean” and then ask the student to say “I like to _____ in the ocean”.
- Sentence completion: A teacher may provide a sentence with a missing word or phrase and ask the student to complete it. For example, a teacher may say “Yesterday, I _____ to the store” and ask the student to fill in the blank with a verb in the past tense.
- Role-play: A teacher or conversation partner may set up a role-play scenario to prompt the learner to use specific language functions or vocabulary. For example, in a restaurant role-play, the teacher may prompt the learner to order food using specific vocabulary and phrases.
These are just a few examples of elicitation techniques that can be used in language teaching and learning. The specific method used may vary depending on the goals of the lesson and the needs of the learner.
Feedback is an important aspect of language development, and its effectiveness depends on a variety of factors, including the type of feedback provided, its timing and appropriateness, and the learners’ individual needs and preferences.
Problem-solving 3
Problem: A group of language learners are struggling with their pronunciation and fluency in their target language. They have been studying for several months but are still having difficulty with speaking and understanding the language.
Chapter summary
Chapter 3 of “Language Development in Children” covers various topics related to language acquisition, including the stages of language development, factors that influence language development, the critical period hypothesis, interactional modifications, and feedback.
The chapter explains the various stages of language development that children go through, including the prelinguistic stage, the holophrastic stage, the telegraphic stage, and the later stages of language development. It also discusses how genetic, environmental, and social factors can influence language development.
One of the most important aspects of language development discussed in this chapter is the critical period hypothesis, which suggests that there is a biologically determined window of opportunity for language acquisition that closes around puberty. The chapter also covers the nature of interactional modifications and how they can help facilitate language learning, as well as different forms of feedback that can aid language development.
To help learners understand the concepts discussed in this chapter, a case study is included along with problem-solving activities. Overall, this chapter provides a comprehensive overview of the different factors that influence language development and how they interact to shape children’s linguistic abilities.
Questions to help review the lesson
- What are the five stages of language acquisition?
- What is the critical period hypothesis in language acquisition?
- How does age affect language acquisition?
- What are the genetic factors that influence language development?
- What are the environmental factors that influence language development?
- What are the social factors that influence language development?
- What are interactional modifications in SLA?
- What is the nature of feedback in language development?
- What are some examples of corrective feedback?
- What are some examples of recasts in language development?
- How does simplifying language aid in language development?
- Can you provide an example of a problem-solving scenario related to language development?
References
Bates, E., Dale, P. S., & Thal, D. (1988). Individual differences and their implications for theories of language development. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Studies of psychosocial risk: The power of longitudinal data (pp. 259-288). Cambridge University Press.
Bishop, D. V. (2002). The role of genes in the etiology of specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 35(4), 311-328.
Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin, 130(6), 858–886.
Bloom, L. (1994). The transition from infancy to language: Acquiring the power of expression. Cambridge University Press.
Bloom, L. (1993). The transition from infancy to language: Acquiring the power of expression. Cambridge University Press.
Bloom, L., & Markson, L. (1998). Capacities underlying word learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(2), 67-73.
Bornstein, M. H., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (1989). Maternal responsiveness and cognitive development in children. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1989(43), 49-61.
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Harvard University Press.
Byers-Heinlein, K., & Lew-Williams, C. (2013). Bilingualism in the early years: What the science says. Brookes Publishing.
Clark, E. V. (2003). First language acquisition. Cambridge University Press. Goswami, U. (2001). Early phonological development and the acquisition of literacy. In M. J.
DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science, 208(4448), 1174-1176.
Dörnyei, Z. (2005). The psychology of the language learner: Individual differences in second language acquisition. Routledge.
Dunn, L. M., & Kontos, S. (1997). Social interaction and language acquisition. In D. Gallimore & R. Tharp (Eds.), Rousing minds to life: Teaching, learning, and schooling in social context (pp. 55-76). Cambridge University Press.
Ellis, R. (2009). Corrective feedback and teacher development. L2 Journal, 1(1), 3-18.
Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2008). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental science, 11(2), 281-291.
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., Pethick, S. J., & Reilly, J. S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5), 1-173.
Flege, J. E. (1987). The production of “new” and “similar” phones in a foreign language: Evidence for the effect of equivalence classification. Journal of Phonetics, 15(1), 47-65.
García-Sierra, A., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Percaccio, C. R., Conboy, B. T., Romo, H., & Klarman, L. (2011). Bilingual language learning: An ERP study relating early brain responses to speech, language input, and later word production. Journal of Phonetics, 39(4), 546-557.
Gass, S. M., & Mackey, A. (2006). Input, interaction, and output in second language acquisition. In Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 389-440). Blackwell Publishing.
Gathercole, V. C. M. (2006). Understanding the development of memory capacity in young children. Erlbaum.
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Butcher, C. (2003). Pointing toward two-word speech in young children. In M. Tomasello (Ed.), The new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure (pp. 43-67). Erlbaum.
Goswami, U. (2001). Early phonological development and the acquisition of literacy. In M. J.
Grossmann, T., Oberecker, R., Koch, S. P., & Friederici, A. D. (2010). The developmental origins of voice processing in the human brain. Neuron, 65(6), 852-858.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Paul H Brookes Publishing.
Hoff, E. (2013). Language development (5th ed.). Wadsworth.
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61(4), 343-365.
Kuhl, P. K. (2010). Brain mechanisms in early language acquisition. Neuron, 67(5), 713-727.
Jia, G., Aaronson, D., & Wu, Y. J. (2019). The effect of immersion education on second language acquisition in young children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(1), 127-144.
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21(1), 60-99.
Krashen, S. D., & Scarcella, R. C. (1978). On the use of the term ‘‘age factor.’’ TESOL Quarterly, 12(4), 439–445.
Kuhl, P. K. (2007). Is speech learning ‘gated’ by the social brain? Developmental Science, 10(1), 110-120.
Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., & Nelson, T. (2008). Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: new data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1493), 979-1000.
Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F. M., & Liu, H. M. (2003). Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(15), 9096-9101.
Kuhl, P. K., & Rivera-Gaxiola, M. (2008). Neural substrates of language acquisition. Annual review of neuroscience, 31, 511-534.
Larsen-Freeman, D., & Long, M. H. (2014). An introduction to second language acquisition research. Routledge.
Law, J., Boyle, J., Harris, F., Harkness, A., & Nye, C. (2003). Prevalence and natural history of primary speech and language delay: Findings from a systematic review of the literature. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38(4), 323-365.
Long, M. H., & Robinson, P. (1998). Focus on form: Theory, research, and practice. In C. Doughty & J. Williams (Eds.), Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition (pp. 15-41). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Lyster, R., & Ranta, L. (1997). Corrective feedback and learner uptake: Negotiation of form in communicative classrooms. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19(1), 37-66.
Lyster, R., & Mori, H. (2006). Interactional feedback and instructional counterbalance. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28(2), 269-300.
Mackey, A., & Goo, J. (2007). Interaction research in SLA: A meta-analysis and research synthesis. In The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition (pp. 451-518). Blackwell Publishing.
National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel. National Institute for Literacy.
Plomin, R., & Kennedy, J. (1999). Estimating and testing the genetic and environmental relationships between learning abilities and disabilities. Annals of Dyslexia, 49, 55-80.
Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. B. (2012). The theory of language socialization. In Handbook of language socialization (pp. 1-21). Springer.
Oller, D. K., Pearson, B. Z., & Cobo-Lewis, A. B. (2007). Profile effects in early bilingual language and literacy. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(2), 191-230.
Oller, D. K. (2000). The emergence of the speech capacity. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
O’Grady, W., Archibald, J., Aronoff, M., & Rees-Miller, J. (2019). Contemporary Linguistics: An Introduction (7th ed.). Bedford/St. Martin’s.
Ota, M., & Saji, N. (2006). Differences in early vocalization between Japanese and American infants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119(5), 3045-3051.
Scerri, T. S., et al. (2011). The DCDC2 deletion is associated with reading and cortical thickness impairments in developmental dyslexia. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 10, 705-713.
Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. A. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: A five-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73(2), 445-460.
Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. National Academies Press.
Swain, M., & Lapkin, S. (2002). Talking it through: Two French immersion learners’ response to reformulation. International Journal of Educational Research, 37(3), 285–304.
Swain, M., & Lapkin, S. (2000). Task-based second language learning: The uses of the first language. Language Teaching Research, 4(3), 251-274.
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Kuchirko, Y., & Tafuro, L. (2014). Language and cognitive development in infancy. Handbook of language and social interaction, 125-140.
Vargha-Khadem, F., et al. (2005). Differential effects of rare deletions and duplications of the FOXP2 gene on speech and language impairment in the general population. Biology of Language, 97(2), 173-180.
van Compernolle, R. A., & Kormos, J. (2014). The role of elicitation in second language acquisition research. Applied Linguistics, 35(2), 168-181.
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (2005). Speech perception as a window into language acquisition. In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language (6th ed., pp. 58-108). John Wiley & Sons.
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2143-2152.
Zimmerman, F. J., Christakis, D. A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). Television and DVD/video viewing in children younger than 2 years. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 161(5), 473-479.
Chapter 3 focuses on the stages of language acquisition and the factors that influence language development. The chapter also discusses the critical period hypothesis, which suggests that there is a biologically determined window of opportunity for language acquisition, and the importance of interactional modifications and feedback in second language acquisition. The chapter begins by discussing the…